RESEARCH ARTICLE


The e-S.F.E.R.A. Questionnaire: A New Tool For Sport Psychologists Working In Mental Training



Andrea Caputo1, *, Stefano Drivet1, Riccardo Sandretto1, Giuseppe Vercelli2, Claudio G. Cortese1
1 Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
2 Sport and Human Performance Psychology Center “Umbro Marcaccioli” (ISEF Foundation), Turin, Italy


Article Metrics

CrossRef Citations:
1
Total Statistics:

Full-Text HTML Views: 1741
Abstract HTML Views: 415
PDF Downloads: 436
ePub Downloads: 249
Total Views/Downloads: 2841
Unique Statistics:

Full-Text HTML Views: 1000
Abstract HTML Views: 279
PDF Downloads: 337
ePub Downloads: 194
Total Views/Downloads: 1810



Creative Commons License
© 2023 Caputo et al.

open-access license: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License (CC-BY 4.0), a copy of which is available at: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode. This license permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

* Address correspondence to this author at the Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy; E-mail: andrea.caputo@unito.it


Abstract

Background:

Esports are gaining worldwide recognition. Given the characteristics of this discipline, esporters need proper mental preparation. The S.F.E.R.A. model is widely used in sport psychology practice in Italy, aiming to maximize athletes’ performance and improve five mental factors: Synchrony, Strength, Energy, Rhythm, and Activation.

Objectives:

The present study aims at 1) developing a questionnaire to detect those factors and 2) investigating their similarity with more classical psychological dimensions.

Methods:

A sample of 211 players of competitive online games filled out the “e-S.F.E.R.A. Questionnaire”. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for each factor and regressions on Engagement, Flow and Addiction were performed.

Results:

Results 1) confirm the monofactorial structures of mental factors and 2) show the positive relations of Energy with Vigor and Dedication of Engagement and Activation with all dimensions considered, including Addiction.

Conclusion:

This study provides sports psychologists with a new framework and tools for esporters’ mental optimization.

Keywords: Mental training, Esports, Sport psychology, Gaming, Sports performance, Eplayers.



1. INTRODUCTION

Esports has become one of the largest industries in terms of value and participation. The number of spectators interested in esports is constantly increasing and has recently reached nearly half a billion globally [1]. Despite this, psychology has long been disinterested in this area, or has focused on its negative aspects, from the effects of exposure to violence to health damage due to a sedentary lifestyle [2]. This has led to a great disparity between the breadth and spread of the gaming industry, on the one hand, and the lack of scientific knowledge about it on the other [3].

In this section, we will make a quick overview of this phenomenon, investigating if and how esports can actually be considered a sports discipline in its own right, exploring the demographic characteristics of the average eplayer today and the physical and psychological effects of this activity. A theoretical and practical model for the optimization of performance will also be introduced, widely used by Italian sports psychologists with athletes of more traditional sports disciplines, and whose applicability to the field of esports will be investigated as the main objective of the present work. First of all, it is necessary to clarify the spelling of the word, as it will be used throughout the paper: following the indications of the Associated Press, the term esport seems to be the right way to refer to this kind of discipline. Therefore other similar forms (Esport, e-sport, etc.) will be left aside [4]. Accordingly, individuals competing in this field will be referred to as esporters or eplayers.

1.1. Can Esports be Considered Sports?

According to Jonasson and Thiborg [5], a sport is a regulated, competitive and physical game [6]. However, this definition has been criticized for being incomplete, given that the criteria of institutionalization [7] and social acceptance [6] were not considered.

As Parry [8] points out, athletic physical activity is not defined by a certain intensity but by whether the action affects the final result or not. This is also the criterion followed by the International Olympic Committee, and it also seems applicable to esports [8]. Institutionalization, on the other hand, is composed of the universalization of the rules and promotion of the sport itself [7]. The creation of common rules is hampered by the fact that the video games on which esports are based have extremely short life spans [9]. To compensate for this problem, esports are progressively moving from a “bottom-up” regulation, that is, from fans and members of the video game community in question to a “top-down” one, where the manufacturer of the game itself, together with its release, is also involved in the organization of its esport scene. Lastly, the social recognition of esports is being affirmed on several levels [6]: on the one hand, from the players, who started defining themselves as athletes since the first video game tournaments in the ‘80s [9]; on the other, from the spectators, whose main motivation to observe this kind of sport is linked to the high competitiveness put on the field by the athletes, similarly to traditional sports’ audiences [10]. Therefore, considering these characteristics, it seems proper to define sport as a physical, competitive, institutionalized, and socially accepted activity.

1.2. Characteristics of an Eplayer and Well-being in Esports

Eplayers can start their careers at a very young age, i.e., as early as 14-15 years old [11], and a sharp decline can already be seen from 24-25 years onwards [12]. Beyond the economic aspect, esporters differ from other people playing video games purely for fun due to the fact that they consider playing video games competitively a job, and engaging in the game differently [13]. The esports market could be considered as a superstar market, characterized by the fact that, similarly to what happens in the traditional sports and entertainment industry, players’ salary and prestige increase exponentially, and not linearly, as the players’ skills and results increase [14].

In the literature, it is highlighted that, for esporters’ careers, spending many hours a day in contact with electronic devices is linked to various physical and psychological consequences.

Physical ones can be grouped into four macro categories:

  • Visual fatigue problems: e.g., Computer Vision Syndrome (Visual fatigue, low back pain, and tension headaches are the most common symptoms [15].
  • Musculoskeletal problems: Caused by maintaining a sitting posture for several hours a day [16].
  • Cardiovascular problems: While the sedentary life of athletes seems to negatively impact blood circulation, on the other hand, the variability of heart rate increases if one competes in esports, improving the body’s ability to adapt to different situations [2].
  • Injury: Another element in common with traditional sports, in the esports field, the most common complications are related to pain in hand or wrist [15].

The consequences on mental health, instead, can be related to:

  • Stress: Linked to increases in blood pressure, respiratory rate, and skin temperature [2].
  • Burnout: In the context of esports, it seems to be correlated with the personality factor of agreeableness, probably given its incompatibility with the particularly aggressive and competitive environment that characterizes esports [17].
  • Addiction: Characterized by three key symptoms, i.e., withdrawal, craving, and tolerance. Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) is the only behavioral addiction, except for gambling addiction, in all of DSM-5. IGD addiction is defined as “persistent and recurring use of the internet to participate in games, often with other players, leading to clinically significant impairment or distress” (DSM-5, pp. 795-798; American Psychiatric Association, 2013), but this conceptualization has been criticized for failing to adequately distinguish between a strong passion for video games and a real addiction [18].

Shifting the focus on the positive aspects, much more related to the sport activity, eplayers can show a number of skills: the ability to interface with artificial intelligence and electronic devices; communication skills; improved cognitive abilities (such as increased visual perception of complex stimuli, better attentional resources, reduced response times, etc. and the ability to endure a long sitting position without compromising performance [11]. Furthermore, it has been observed that some eplayers habitually employ some psychological techniques during their gaming sessions in order to increase their performance [19].

A number of studies have shown that psychological training skills can lead to improved esports performance. For example, psychological skill training (PST) to improve some mental techniques such as goal setting, self-talk, mental imagery, mental rehearsal and relaxation seem to have positive effects on team cohesion and performance [20, 21] and even manuals about psychological training for esporters are also being developed [22]. More generally, it has been observed that higher psychological skills and higher self-regulation and social support are linked to better esport performance [23]. Psychological training techniques are habitually employed in traditional sports psychology, such as those focused on mental toughness and stress-coping processes, having a beneficial effect in this regard, also for esporters [24].

In light of these findings, this paper will be focused on the development of an instrument useful to sports psychologists to monitor some psychological aspects which could enhance esport performance, based on an Italian model of psychological training, already widely used in traditional sports practice, adapting it to the particular features of esports.

1.3. The S.F.E.R.A. Model

The present research is based on the “S.F.E.R.A. model”, developed by Vercelli [25, 26]. It is widely used in sports practice, adopted by the Italian National Olympic Committee (CONI), by several Olympic athletes and even by some professional clubs of various sports. According to the model, there are five mental factors to train to improve performance.

  • Synchrony: The ability of being fully present in what one is doing at the moment of performance, supported by techniques such as self-talk and self-observation. It is the key to reaching all one's resources, especially the unconscious ones, and making them available to the performance. In fact, it is common for athletes who have reached this state to report that they move almost automatically, as if they were not driving their own bodies.
  • Strength: To bring only and exclusively the athlete's strengths into the competition (physical, technical, and psychological skills and abilities that the athlete acknowledges possessing in an excellent way). It is linked to the concept of self-efficacy. Any defects should be temporarily forgotten. It is important to emphasize that this attitude is functional only at the moment of the game [27], while during training, athletes should work to their limits.
  • Energy: The active use and regulation of strength and power, which allows athletes to best express their resources in a self-controlled way. It is further divided into Dynamism (enthusiasm and be ready to face unexpected events) and Dominance (assertiveness on opponents and influence on teammates).
  • Rhythm: The orderly succession of time intervals, which generates the right flow in the sequence of movements and gives quality to the action.
  • Activation: A motivational engine of an emotional and passionate nature, favored by connection with one's emotions, self-talk, and self-analysis. Activation is, more than any other factor, an internal sensation, the athletes’ belief that they are ready to perform in the match.

To achieve maximum performance, there must be a strengthening and a total balance between these five factors. Furthermore, the mind and body, conscious and unconscious, must work in unison, as if they were a unity, clearing the mind of any other thought.

Subsequently, this theory was enriched by qualitative methods of analysis and graphical visualization of the five factors, such as the S.F.E.R.A. mandala [25, 27] and quantitative methods, such as various questionnaires suited to different contexts of sport life: individual activity, group sports, training and competition, etc. [28].

Some similarities are noticeable between the theoretical descriptions of such factors and several classical constructs more common in sport psychology. For example, the pleasant dimension of Activation and the automaticity of thought and movement described in Synchrony closely resemble the dimension of Flow at Sport, and especially its subdimension of Absorption [29]. On the other hand, the focus on effort and physicality of Rhythm and Energy reminds us of the construct of Engagement, and of its Vigor subdimension in particular [30]. Thus, considering the use of the S.F.E.R.A. model applied in the esports’ field, it seems possible to find relations between Flow and Synchrony with Activation, and between Engagement and Rhythm with Energy, other than Activation again, as also the construct of Engagement includes an emotional aspect [30]. Moreover, as the five factors of S.F.E.R.A. sustain the healthy development of one's passion, a negative or absent relation with addiction is expected.

1.4. Objectives

Consequently, the present research pursues the following goals:

  • To detect the five S.F.E.R.A. factors in gamers by developing the “e-S.F.E.R.A. questionnaire” and to measure its statistical properties;
  • To explore the relationships of the five e-S.F.E.R.A. factors with more classical psychological dimensions implied in sport psychology, namely, Engagement, Flow at Sport and Gameaholism.

2. METHODS

2.1. Participants and Procedures

The study was conducted with a convenient sampling method, recruiting respondents by e-mail or online word of mouth, spreading the questionnaire in guilds and groups of different kinds of games. The main Italian esport scenes have been contacted: the Italian Esport Prototype Championship (Machine Simulator Tournament), the PG Nationals (Italian League of Legends Championship), the eSerie A Tim and the Italian Fortnite Championship. The final sample size was 211 people, of which 78.2% were males. Their average age was 26.5 years, and they devoted, on average, 15.32 hours to video games per week. 21.3% participate in official competitions. Shooter games and MMORPGs (massively multiplayer online role-playing games) were the most popular genres (23.8% and 20% of respondents, respectively), followed by i-racing (14.8%), MOBA (multiplayer online battle arena, chosen by 11.4% of respondents) and card games (10%). Finally, the last demographic question was dedicated to the self-perceived level of professionalism: 94 people rated themselves as casual players (44.5%), defined as someone who games just amateurly, for fun; 85 as intermediate (40.3%), occasionally participating in ranked matches; and 32 as advanced (15.2%), someone who participates in high-level competitions.

It is important to underline the discrepancy that can be seen between a self-assessment of respondents’ expertise level (only 15.2% define themselves as an advanced player) and a relative behavioral indicator (as many as 21.3% have participated in official competitions). These results show how difficult it is to clearly define who is and who is not an eplayer [31].

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the sample.
Gender Male 78.2%
Female 21.8%
Age 18-25 53.6%
26-30 23.7%
31-40 17.5%
> 40 5.2%
Favourite game genre Shooter 23.8%
MMORPG 20%
iRacing 14.8%
MOBA 11.4%
Cards 10%
Sports 7.1%
Fighting 3.3%
Other 9.5%
Professionalism level Casual 44.5%
Intermediate 40.3%
Advanced 15.2%
Participation to official competitions Yes 21.3%
No 78.7%

2.2. Instruments

e-S.F.E.R.A: The focal point of the research was the development of the e-S.F.E.R.A. Questionnaire. Items were formulated by two experts (a full professor of applied psychology and an expert sport psychologist certified by the International Olympic Committee). The questionnaire was created and then administered in Italian, following previous questionnaires based on S.F.E.R.A. models related to sports performance [32], business [33] and university teaching areas [34]. Both negatively and positively worded items were formulated to avoid acquiescence or agreement bias [35]. A starting pool of 83 items (18 for Synchrony, 17 for Strengths, 15 for Energy, 16 for Rhythm, and 17 for Activation) was created. During the next step, as suggested by the procedure described by DeVellis (2016), two experts (i.e., sport psychologists who daily use the S.F.E.R.A. model in their psychological practice with athletes) were involved to evaluate if items could properly refer to each dimension they were created for, based on their knowledge and experience. A total of 23 items (6 from Synchrony, 5 from Strengths, 3 from Energy, 4 from Rhythm, and 5 from Activation) were deleted from the starting pool, following experts’ suggestions. A 12-item scale was then reachedfor each factor of the model, to capture their main aspects. Moreover, to evaluate its face validity and ensure the comprehensibility of the item formulation, a pilot administration of the questionnaire was carried out, involving some ex esporters or players of competitive video games without participating in tournaments. Some items’ formulations were adjusted following their suggestions, in order to be more understandable and transversal, i.e. usable by esporters who play different types of games; for example, the item “I moved my avatar with the right timing” was reformulated as “I moved with the right timing”, since not every game has got a single avatar to control. The 60-items version of the instrument was used for the analysis in this work.

The questionnaire requires thinking about a competitive gaming contest that the player recently participated in and responding to a series of statements related to it. The response mode is a Likert scale from 1 (Completely disagree) to 5 (Completely agree).

Flow at Sport: Flow is defined as “a state of consciousness generated while participating in an activity which enjoys and absorbs the individual, and which is intrinsically rewarding” [29]. The I-WOLFS scale evolved from the Work-Related Flow Inventory, originally developed by Bakker [36], which intends to measure Flow within work contexts. Bakker further operationalized this construct into three dimensions: Absorption, an immersive experience where all attentional resources are focused on the current task, even forgetting the passage of time or surrounding events; Work Enjoyment, defined as the subjective pleasure felt during the activity; and Intrinsic Motivation, the desire to execute a task exclusively because of the pleasure directly coming from it [29]. In 2018, Zito and colleagues [29] adapted this scale to the sports context, creating the I-WOLFS scale. Thus, Work Enjoyment has reconceptualized into Sport Enjoyment and Intrinsic Motivation became Intrinsic Sport Motivation. This scale was further modified for this study, to make it adequate to the esportive world, and the items were suitably rewarded. Also in this case, the response mode is a Likert scale, ranging from 0 (Completely disagree) to 6 (Completely agree).

2.2.1. Engagement

This dimension was adapted from the Utrecht Work Engagement Scale (UWES) [37], which intends to measure engagement at work, defined as a positive mental state of pleasure and satisfaction toward one’s job [30]. It can be divided into three dimensions: Absorption, which, similar to the previous scale’s dimension of the same name, is defined as being fully concentrated and happily engrossed in one’s work, whereby time passes quickly; Vigor, defined as the grade of effort, energy and resilience invested into the activity; and Dedication, the subjective feeling of the meaning of one’s work, generating pride and enthusiasm [30]. To adapt this instrument to the field of video gaming, the items of its Italian version [30] have been reworded, but its structure has remained almost identical. In this case, the response mode is a frequency scale ranging from 0 (never) to 6 (always).

2.2.2. Gameaholism

Workaholism is defined as a sort of addiction to work, divided into three dimensions: Work Involvement, the inability to disconnect from work and to relax; Drive, the frequency and intensity of obsessive thoughts relative to work; and Enjoyment of Work, the positive subjective experiences felt during one’s work [38]. The Gameholism scale was created for this study starting from the Workaholism Battery [38], but, differently from it doesn’t measure the three subdimensions separately. The response mode is a Likert Scale ranging from 1 (Completely disagree) to 5 (Completely agree).

2.3. Data Analysis

Analyses were performed using both SPSS 28 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) and Mplus 8 (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, USA) software. SPSS 28 was implied for: descriptive statistics, reliability (Cronbach’s ɑ), correlations, and regression. Mplus 8 was used to perform CFAs with a Maximum Likelihood method of extraction. CFA (confirmatory factor analysis) was used to test the mono-factorial structure of each S.F.E.R.A. factor; the rationale behind this choice is linked to the practical purpose of this research i.e. to provide sports psychologists with tools to be used separately during psychological sessions aimed at evaluating and optimizing a specific factor. For the sake of completeness, indices of the 5-factor model will also be presented, both for all initial items and those remaining after individual factor analyses.

Common method bias was assessed with Harman’s single factor test [39]. Significant common method variance (CMV) exists if one general factor accounts for the majority of covariance in the measures. Therefore, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was performed to test this eventuality. All variables in this study were included in the EFA. Analyses bore out twenty-one factors with eigenvalues greater than one accounting for 62.21% of the total variance, while the first and second factors accounted for 7.28 and 19.29% of the variance, respectively. Considering these results, not a single factor is emerging and accounting for most of the variance, showing the lack of a common method issue.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

The factors measured by the e-S.F.E.R.A. Questionnaire showed the following mean points: 3.56 (SD = 0.70) for the Synchrony factor, 3.55 (SD = 0.67) for Strength, 3.59 (SD = 0.63) for Energy, 3.49 (SD = 0.73) for the Rhythm, and 3.26 (SD = 0.90) for the Activation.

Multivariate and univariate skewness and kurtosis of all 60 items were calculated. For multivariate skewness and kurtosis, the tool at the site https:// webpower.psychstat.org/ models/ kurtosis was used (Mardia’s multivariate kurtosis z = 24.36; p < .001); an acceptable value of Mardia’s normalized multivariate kurtosis coefficient is less than 3.0 [40]. Therefore this value shows a non-normal distribution of data. On the other hand, values below ±2.0 for skewness and ±7.0 for kurtosis, according to a more liberal standard [40], are acceptable and the condition of univariate normality. According to these cut-offs, univariate skewness and kurtosis show a normal distribution for all items except for item 4 of Activation (skewness = –2.05).

Table 2. Univariate descriptive statistics of all items.
- Item Mean SD Asymmetry Kurtosis
Stats SE Stats SE
Synchrony Sync_1 3.31 .99 .05 .17 –.37 .33
Sync_2 2.73 1.11 .17 .17 –.83 .33
Sync_3 3.65 .90 –.09 .17 –.41 .33
Sync_4 3.84 .99 –.75 .17 .21 .33
Sync_5 3.16 .89 –.12 .17 –.09 .33
Sync_6 3.50 1.02 –.37 .17 –.24 .33
Sync_7 3.45 1.24 –.41 .17 –.90 .33
Sync_8 3.34 1.29 –.27 .17 –1.04 .33
Sync_9 3.67 .99 –.33 .17 –.39 .33
Sync_10 4.03 1.07 –1.15 .17 .76 .33
Sync_11 3.80 1.01 –.63 .17 –.19 .33
Sync_12 3.92 1.09 –.96 .17 .38 .33
Strength Str_1 3.78 .91 –.34 .17 –.15 .33
Str_2 3.27 .99 –.08 .17 –.46 .33
Str_3 3.08 1.16 –.11 .17 –.93 .33
Str_4 4.06 .86 –1.24 .17 2.18 .33
Str_5 3.71 .84 –.25 .17 –.23 .33
Str_6 3.61 .98 –.24 .17 –.43 .33
Str_7 3.37 1.15 –.32 .17 –.90 .33
Str_8 3.55 .82 .09 .17 –.28 .33
Str_9 3.99 1.15 –1 .17 .12 .33
Str_10 3.28 .92 –.16 .17 –.18 .33
Str_11 3.62 .9 –.29 .17 –.14 .33
Str_12 3.36 1.02 –.61 .17 .001 .33
Energy Ene_1 3.57 .98 –.12 .17 –.72 .33
Ene_2 3.66 1.03 –.83 .17 .23 .33
Ene_3 4.06 .85 –.87 .17 .62 .33
Ene_4 3.37 1.08 –.12 .17 –.67 .33
Ene_5 3.41 .91 –.02 .17 .05 .33
Ene_6 3.91 .89 –.39 .17 –.48 .33
Ene_7 4.28 .97 –1.54 .17 2.18 .33
Ene_8 2.92 1.07 –.01 .17 –.59 .33
Ene_9 3.37 1.14 –.47 .17 –.52 .33
Ene_10 3.22 .92 –.05 .17 .18 .33
Ene_11 3.99 1.01 –.99 .17 .65 .33
Ene_12 2.85 1.2 .08 .17 –.85 .33
Rhythm Rhy_1 3.41 1.12 –.43 .17 –.45 .33
Rhy_2 3.80 1.02 –.6 .17 –.05 .33
Rhy_3 4.06 1 –1.06 .17 .58 .33
Rhy_4 3.77 1.06 –.71 .17 –.09 .33
Rhy_5 3.45 .92 –.09 .17 –.36 .33
Rhy_6 3.35 .95 –.16 .17 –.06 .33
Rhy_7 3.48 1.16 –.56 .17 –.42 .33
Rhy_8 3.08 .88 –.03 .17 .15 .33
Rhy_9 4.21 .97 –1.23 .17 .97 .33
Rhy_10 3.25 1.08 –.26 .17 –.41 .33
Rhy_11 3.76 1.06 –.73 .17 –.09 .33
Rhy_12 3.46 .96 –.28 .17 –.14 .33
Activation Act_1 3.23 1.17 –.18 .17 –.76 .33
Act_2 4.16 1.06 –1.33 .17 1.27 .33
Act_3 2.18 1.18 .82 .17 –.2 .33
Act_4 4.49 .87 –2.05 .17 4.07 .33
Act_5 3.60 1.07 –.3 .17 –.62 .33
Act_6 4.08 1.06 –1.16 .17 .68 .33
Act_7 3.03 1.22 –.02 .17 –.86 .33
Act_8 2.64 1.26 .34 .17 –.89 .33
Act_9 3.29 1.13 –.15 .17 –.70 .33
Act_10 4.43 .85 –1.79 .17 3.50 .33
Act_11 3.77 1.02 –.49 .17 –.17 .33
Act_12 3.74 1.02 –.42 .17 –.39 .33

3.2. Exploratory Factor Analysis

An EFA (with maximum likelihood method of extraction) was performed for each of the dimensions of the model. In this paragraph will be presented the range of loadings alongside the values of internal consistency (Cronbach’s ɑ) for each dimension of the initial 60-item version of the e-S.F.E.R.A. Questionnaire.

For Synchrony, items explained 21.65% of the variance, with loadings ranging from –.08 to .73 (Cronbach’s ɑ = .69). As regards Strengths, items explained 34.65% of the variance, with loadings ranging from .01 to .78 (ɑ = .81). For Energy, items explained 22.53% of the variance, with loadings ranging from .07 to .73 (Cronbach’s ɑ = .71). For Rhythm, items explained 38.70% of the variance, with loadings ranging from .28 to .81 (Cronbach’s ɑ = .87). Lastly, for Activation, items explained 36.67% of the variance, with loadings ranging from .43 to .79 (Cronbach’s ɑ = .86).

3.3. Confirmatory Factor Analysis

For each single factor, CFA maximum likelihood (ML) estimation was used, except for the Activation factor, where the robust method of estimation (MRL) was used as an alternative [41], considering the non-normal distribution of all items. For the same reasons, the MLR method was used for the five-factor CFAs.

For each CFA of this work, the following cut-offs of the fit indices were followed: values of Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) > .90/.95 indicate a good fit of the model, as well as values of Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) < .05/.08 indicate an acceptable fit (while scores <.10 indicate a mediocre one) and of Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) < .08 [42].

The CFA of the starting 12-item scale of Synchrony (M1) showed a not good fit to the data: χ2(54) = 271.792; p <.001; CFI = .60; TLI = .51; RMSEA = .14 (CI:0.12, 0.15); SRMR = .11. Therefore, following the modification indices, alternative models were tried, deleting the following items: Sync_2 (M2); Sync_7 (M3); Sync_8 (M4); Sync_1 (M5); Sync_4 (M6); Sync_12 (M7); Sync_10 (M8). The last model shows the best fit (Table 3). Loadings ranged from .52 to .75 (Fig. 1).

Fig. (1). CFA (maximum likelihood (ML) estimation) standardized solution for the Synchrony factor.

The CFA of the starting 12-item scale of Strength (M1) showed the following not-good-enough fit to the data: χ2(54)= 209.732; p <.001; CFI= .83; TLI= .79; RMSEA= .12 (CI:0.10, 0.13); SRMR= .08. Thus, following the modification indices, alternative models were tried, deleting the following items: Str_12 (M2); Str_3 (M3); Str_4 (M4); Str_5 (M5); Str_6 (M6); and testing the model with the correlation between items Str_9 and Str_7 (M7). The last model shows the best fit (Table 4), with loadings ranging from .36 and .79 (Fig. 2).

Fig. (2). CFA (maximum likelihood (ML) estimation) standardized solution for the Strength factor.

The CFA of the starting 12-item scale of Energy (M1) showed the following not good fit to the data: χ2(54) = 313.924; p <.001; CFI= .57; TLI= .47; RMSEA = .15 (CI:0.13, 0.17); SRMR= .12. Thus, following the modification indices, alternative models were tried, deleting the following items: Ene_9 (M2); Ene_12 (M3); Ene_8 (M4); Ene_2 (M5); Ene_11 (M6); Ene_7 (M7); and testing the model with the correlation between items Ene_4 and Ene_1 (M8). The last model shows the best fit (Table 5), with loadings ranging from .40 and .82 (Fig. 3).

Table 3. Results of alternative Confirmatory Factor Analysis model of the Synchrony factor.
χ2 df p CFI TLI RMSEA SRMR Δχ2 (df) p
M1 271.792 54 < .001 .60 .51 .14 (.12, .15) .11
M2 235.933 44 < .001 .63 .54 .14 (.13, .16) .11 35.859 (10) <.00001
M3 193.244 35 < .001 .67 .58 .15 (13, .17) .10 42.689 (9) <.00001
M4 149.252 27 < .001 .73 .64 .15 (.12, .17) .09 43.992 (8) < .00001
M5 141.854 20 < .001 .72 .61 .17 (.14, .20) .10 7.398 (7) <.00001
M6 123.940 14 < .001 .73 .60 .19 (.16, .22) .11 17.914 (6) <.00001
M7 62.894 9 < .001 .84 .73 .17 (.13, .21) .08 61.046 (5) <.00001
M8 10.689 5 .06 .98 .96 .07 (.00, .13) .03 52.205 (4) <.00001
Notes: M1: Starting 12-item 1-factor model; M2: M1 without item 2; M3: M2 without item 7; M4: M3 without item 8;
M5: M4 without item 1; M6: M5 without item 4; M7: M6 without item 12; M8: M7 without item 10.
Table 4. Results of alternative confirmatory factor analysis model of the Strength factor.
χ2 df p CFI TLI RMSEA SRMR Δχ2 (df) p
M1 209.732 54 < .001 .83 .79 .12 (.10, .13) .08
M2 164.342 44 < .001 .86 .82 .11 (.10, .13) .08 45.390 (10) < .00001
M3 152.259 35 < .001 .83 .78 .13 (.11, .15) .08 12.083 (9) < .00001
M4 124.471 27 < .001 .83 .78 .13 (.11, .15) .08 27.788 (8) < .00001
M5 69.416 20 < .001 .90 .87 .11 (.08, .14) .07 55.055 (7) < .00001
M6 63.336 14 < .001 .90 .84 .13 (.10, .16) .07 6.08 (6) < .00001
M7 27.855 13 < .01 .97 .95 .07 (.03, .11) .04 35.481 (1) < .00001
Notes: M1: Starting 12-item 1-factor model; M2: M1 without item 12; M3: M2 without item 3; M4: M3 without item 4;
M5: M4 without item 5; M6: M5 without item 6; M7: M6 correlating items 9 and 7.
Table 5. Results of alternative confirmatory factor analysis model of the energy factor.
χ2 df p CFI TLI RMSEA SRMR Δχ2 (df) p
M1 313.924 54 < .001 .57 .47 .15 (.13, .17) .12
M2 280.813 44 < .001 .60 .14 .16 (.14, .18) .12 33.111 (10) < .00001
M3 239.795 35 < .001 .62 .52 .17 (.15, .19) .11 41.018 (9) < .00001
M4 216.700 27 < .001 .64 .52 .18 (.16, .20) .11 23.095 (8) < .00001
M5 155.994 20 < .001 .70 .58 .18 (.15, .21) .10 60.706 (7) < .00001
M6 77.482 14 < .001 .83 .74 .15 (.12, .18) .08 78.512 (6) < .00001
M7 44.739 9 < .001 .88 .80 .14 (.10, .18) .06 32.743 (5) < .00001
M8 23.286 8 < .01 .95 .91 .09 (.05, .14) .04 21.453 (1) < .00001
Notes: M1: Starting 12-item 1-factor model; M2: M1 without item 9; M3: M2 without item 12; M4: M3 without item 8;
M5: M4 without item 2; M6: M5 without item 11; M7: M6 without item 7; M8: M7 correlating items 4 and 1.
Fig. (3). CFA (maximum likelihood (ML) estimation) standardized solution for the Energy factor.

The CFA of the starting 12-item scale of Rhythm (M1) showed the following not-good-enough fit to the data: χ2(54) = 273.962; p <.001; CFI= .79; TLI= .74; RMSEA = .14 (CI:0.12, 0.15); SRMR= .09. Thus, following the modification indices, alternative models were tried, deleting the following items: Rhy_1 (M2); Rhy_7 (M3); Rhy_9 (M4); Rhy_11 (M5); Rhy_4 (M6); and testing the model with the correlation between items Rhy_3 and Rhy_2 (M7). The last model shows the best fit (Table 6), with loadings ranging from .57 and .82 (Fig. 4).

Fig. (4). CFA (maximum likelihood (ML) estimation) standardized solution for the Rhythm factor.

The CFA of the starting 12-item scale of Activation (M1) showed the following not-good-enough fit to the data: χ2(54) = 430.961; p <.001; CFI= .66; TLI= .59; RMSEA = .18 (CI:0.17, 0.20); SRMR= .12. Thus, following the modification indices, alternative models were tried, deleting the following items: Act_3 (M2); Act_2 (M3); Act_10 (M4); Act_6 (M5); Act_4 (M6); Act_1 (M7); Act_12 (M8); and testing the model with the correlation between items Act_8 and Act_5 (M9). The last model shows the best fit (Table 7), with loadings ranging from .59 and .86 (Fig. 5).

Table 6. Results of alternative confirmatory factor analysis model of the rhythm factor.
χ2 df p CFI TLI RMSEA SRMR Δχ2 (df) p
M1 273.962 54 < .001 .79 .74 .14 (.12, .15) .09
M2 211.543 44 < .001 .83 .78 .13 (.12, .15) .08 62.419 (10) < .00001
M3 172.058 35 < .001 .85 .81 .14 (.12, .16) .07 39.485 (9) < .00001
M4 140.007 27 < .001 .87 .83 .14 (.12, .16) .06 32.051 (8) < .00001
M5 102.082 20 < .001 .90 .86 .14 (.11, .17) .05 37.925 (7) < .00001
M6 54.384 14 < .001 .94 .91 .12 (.08, .15) .04 47.698 (6) < .00001
M7 38.577 13 < .001 .96 .94 .10 (.06, .13) .04 15.807 (1) < .00001
Notes: M1: Starting 12-item 1-factor model; M2: M1 without item 1; M3: M2 without item 7; M4: M3 without item 9;
M5: M4 without item 11; M6: M5 without item 4; M7: M6 correlating items 3 and 2.
Table 7. Results of alternative confirmatory factor analysis model of the activation factor.
χ2 df p CFI TLI RMSEA SRMR Δχ2 (df) p
M1 350.693 54 < .001 .64 .56 .16 (.15, .18) .12
M2 314.812 44 < .001 .64 .56 .17 (.15, .19) .11 35.881 (10) < .00001
M3 256.172 35 < .001 .67 .58 .17 (.15, .19) .11 58.64 (9) < .00001
M4 175.486 27 < .001 .74 .66 .16 (.14, .18) .09 80.686 (8) < .00001
M5 103.580 20 < .001 .82 .76 .14 (.11, .17) .08 71.906 (7) < .00001
M6 80.084 14 < .001 .85 .78 .15 (.12, .18) .07 23.496 (6) < .00001
M7 56.556 9 < .001 .87 .79 .16 (.12, .20) .06 23.528 (5) < .00001
M8 13.591 5 < .05 .97 .94 .09 (.03, .15) .03 42.965 (4) < .00001
Notes: M1: Starting 12-item 1-factor model; M2: M1 without item 3; M3: M2 without item 2; M4: M3 without item 10;
M5: M4 without item 6; M6: M5 without item 4; M7: M6 without item 1; M8: M7 without item 12.
Fig. (5). CFA (maximum likelihood (MLR) estimation) standardized solution for the Activation factor.

Lastly, Table 8 shows the fit indices of the 5-factor models, the first with all 60 items and the second with the remaining 30 items. In the Appendix, we propose an English version of the questionnaire, but future studies need to perform a back translation process to confirm this version [43].

3.4. Relations between the Study Dimensions

Table 9 highlights strong correlations between the five S.F.E.R.A. factors and all other dimensions, the sole exception is Gameaholism, which seems to correlate with Activation only.


Table 8. Comparison of fit indices of the CFAs on the 5-factor model
- χ2 df p CFI TLI RMSEA SRMR
M1 7236.430 1770 < .001 .59 .57 .08 (.08, .08) .11
M2 3030.857 435 < .001 .86 .84 .07 (.06, .07) .08
Notes: M1: Starting 60-item 5-factor model; M2: Final 30-item 5-factor model.

Table 9. Correlations between study variables.
- ɑ 1a 1b 1c 1d 1e 2a 2b 2c 2d 3a 3b 3c 3d 4
1a Synchrony .77 1
1b Strength .80 .64*** 1
1c Energy .76 .63*** .68*** 1
1d Rhythm .88 .65*** .77*** .72*** 1
1e Activation .85 .45*** .27*** .34*** .34*** 1
2a Flow Enjoyment .92 .42*** .35*** .38*** .36*** .60*** 1
2b Flow Int. Motivation .50 .28*** .22** .25*** .24** .45*** .53*** 1
2c Flow
Absorption
.88 .31*** .14* .27*** .22** .46*** .52*** .69*** 1
2d Flow
Total
.90 .39*** .27*** .34*** .31*** .58*** .77*** .88*** .89*** 1
3a Engagement
Vigor
.77 .46*** .39*** .45*** .38*** .58*** .64*** .60*** .63*** .73*** 1
3b Engagement
Dedication
.83 .47*** .38*** .45*** .40*** .68*** .70*** .49*** .57*** .67*** .73*** 1
3c Engagement
Absorption
.87 .37*** .21** .30*** .26*** .55*** .56*** .71*** .84*** .84*** .78*** .68*** 1
3d Engagement
Total
.92 .48*** .36*** .43*** .38*** .67*** .69*** .67*** .75*** .83*** .92*** .88*** .91*** 1
4 Gameaholism .76 .06 –.02 –.04 –.05 .27*** .19** .16* .25*** .23** .30*** .26*** .35*** .34*** 1
Note: * p < .05, ** p < .01; *** p < .001. First column shows Cronbach’s ɑ.
Table 10. Results of regressions of the e-S.F.E.R.A. factors on Engagement, Flow at Sport and Gameaholism.
Engagement Flow at Sport Game-
aholism
Vigor Dedic. Absor. Total Enjoy. Int. Motiv. Abs. Total
R2 .42 .52 .33 .50 .41 .21 .24 .37 .10
Synchrony .09 .05 .14 .11 .05 .02 .13 .09 .05
Strength .14 .09 –.05 .06 .12 .05 –.18 –.01 .05
Energy .21* .17* .11 .18* .09 .06 .16 .12 –.10
Rhythm –.10 –.03 –.04 –.06 –.01 .00 .02 .01 –.17
Activation .46*** .58*** .048*** .56*** .52*** .40*** .38*** .50*** .32***
Note: * p < .05, ** p < .01; *** p < .001. The columns show the dependent variables of the regression analyses. The second row indicates the subdimensions of Engagement and Flow at Sport, respectively: Vigor, Dedic. (=Dedication) and Absorb. (Absorption) for Engagement; Enjoy. (=Sport Enjoyment), Int. Motiv. (Internal Motivation) and Abs. (Absorption) for Flow at Sport.

As regard Cronbach’s ɑ, all dimensions show good internal consistency, except the Intrinsic Sport Motivation subdimension of Flow at Sport. Only two out of five items were selected for this dimension, considering that not all items could fit the characteristics of the sample. Specifically, items related to the motivation to play games in free time, which is tautological, and related to the motivation to play linked to retribution (the research was open to both professional and casual players) were not included, choosing instead those that cover the semantic perimeter of the factor.

Regression analysis (Table 10) shows more nuanced relations: only Activation has a strong influence on the dependent variables, including Gameaholism, while Energy has an influence over the Engagement subdimensions of Vigor and Dedication.

4. DISCUSSION

Demographic data revealed the importance of video games as a source of entertainment: on average, more than 15 hours a week are spent gaming. The population interested in gaming appears still characterized by a strong prevalence of the male gender over the female one.

Following the study's objectives, firstly, the good fit of mono-factorial structures of the e-S.F.E.R.A. factors suggest that these tools could be used independently of each other by sports mental coaches in order to monitor during psychological sessions; this conclusion seems to be also supported by the not-so-good fit indices of the two 5-factor models. When creating and formulating items, it was always assessed if each covered the factor's semantic meaning. Item wording sometimes seemed to repeat themselves but in different ways or with reverse items. Indeed, almost all items that fit best within the model were precisely those that are not worded in reverse. Moreover, the final version items seem to cover the entire semantic range well and retrace the definition of each factor.

As regards the second objective, it is possible to notice the positive link between the Energy factor and the subdimensions of Vigor and Dedication of Engagement. All three of these constructs, in fact, have in common the elements of physical effort and proactivity: for the S.F.E.R.A. model, Energy represents the active use of force, as well as its regulation, but also enthusiasm and readiness, contained in the subdimension of Dynamism. Similarly, for Engagement, Vigor represents the degree of commitment, energy, and resilience invested in one’s activity while Dedication the enthusiasm felt toward it.

Another noteworthy result is the strong relationship observed between the e-S.F.E.R.A. factor of Activation and all dependent variables. Since Activation is described as the emotive and passionate component of motivation, this result could mean that pleasure generated from one's activity and sincere passion toward it may be an important predictor of one's effort and satisfaction, as described by the various subdimensions of Flow and Engagement.

However, a positive relation is also observable between Activation and Gameaholism, thus highlighting a possible dark side of this construct: the passion and dedication felt towards a gaming activity could also be a prelude to the potential development of a video game addiction. As pleasure is central to the construct of activation, it is plausible that it also relates to addictions, as they are founded on the repetitive overstimulation of the brain pleasure center, the Nucleus Accumbens [44].

LIMITATIONS

Before ending the discussion, it is important to dwell on the limitations of the study. Firstly, the convenience sample: the investigation was open to anyone playing an online competitive video game; therefore, numerous non-professional gamers also contributed. Thus, this study should not be considered as completely focused on professional esporters, but a starting point for investigating the mental factors that impact and could improve performance in the context of competitive video gaming. Furthermore, the low amount of professionals and the heterogeneity of our sample prevent us to generalise results for the gamer population. Then, a number of different games and different genres were considered. Although all games considered share the competitive aspect, it was not possible to make a clear distinction between which is actually an esport and which is not.

FUTURE DEVELOPMENTS

The results suggest the importance of future research to investigate which other classical psychological dimensions the S.F.E.R.A. factors of Synchrony, Rhythm and Strength could be linked to. For example, the latter may show some meeting points with the construct of self-efficacy, defined as an individual's belief in their capacity to act in the ways necessary to reach specific goals [45, 46] and directly referred to in the original model’s definition of this dimension [25]. Furthermore, as S.F.E.R.A. is a model ultimately designed to reach maximum performance in sports competitions [47], the relation between its factors and actual performance indicators (such as victories in tournaments or different kinds of scores) should be investigated.

Contextually, future research could highlight longitudinal fluctuations in S.F.E.R.A. dimensions scores in professional eplayers before and after taking part in a program of psychological optimization with professional sports psychologists.

Moreover, the apparent strong relation of Activation to pleasure and passion toward an activity, and the risk of developing an addiction to it, could be more deeply investigated. Finally, the difference in the percentage points between respondents who claim to have participated in an official competition and those who define themselves as advanced players show how blurry the boundary is between who is and who is not an eplayer. As an exploratory study, this study did not follow such a specific goal, but if future research wants to focus on eplayers only, then the characteristics of professional eplayers should be defined more strictly.

CONCLUSION

The results of this research are a mirror of reality. The esports phenomenon is emerging in an increasingly important way in recent times and especially in Asia, less so in Italy. Professionalism in esports in Italy is still not so widespread, where there is also a high turnover of players within teams. Considering, however, the evolution of the phenomenon, it could be wise for sports psychologists to be equipped with all the necessary tools, skills, and preparation to be able to support this kind of athlete that is emerging so quickly, even in the Italian context, albeit at a different pace (or “Rhythm”, speaking in the S.F.E.R.A. slang).

Participants gave their written informed consent to participate in this study.

ETHICS APPROVAL AND CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE

Ethical review and approval were not required for the study on human participants per local legislation and institutional requirements.

HUMAN AND ANIMAL RIGHTS

No animals were used for studies that are the basis of this research. All the humans were used in accordance with the ethical standards of the committee responsible for human experimentation (institutional and national), and >with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2013 (http://ethics.iit.edu/ecodes/node/3931).

CONSENT FOR PUBLICATION

Participation in the study was anonymous and voluntary, and all participants were adults.

STANDARDS OF REPORTING

STROBE guidelines were followed.

AVAILABILITY OF DATA AND MATERIALS

The data that support the findings of this study are available within the article.

FUNDING

None.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors report there are no competing interests to declare.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors would like to thank: the expert sports psychologists Dr. Matteo Gabiano and Dr. Manuel Toscano for their feedback during the item formulation; the expert esporters and gamers Dr. Ginevra Cataldi and Dr. Elena Del Fante for their feedback on item adjustment; and Dr. Barbara Cerato for the recruitment of eplayers and gamers into the study.

APPENDIX. English and Italian final versions of the e-S.F.E.R.A. Questionnaire.
Factor English version
During the game that I thought about…
Italian version
Durante la partita a cui ho pensato…
Synchrony Sync_3 Thoughts and actions coincided Pensieri e azioni coincidevano
Sync_5 I felt I had everything under control Sentivo di avere tutto sotto controllo
Sync_6 I had in mind all actions to be carried out “step by step” Avevo in mente le azioni da svolgere “passo dopo passo”
Sync_9 I could effortlessly perceive the signals coming from the game situation Riuscivo a percepire senza sforzo i segnali che provenivano dalla situazione di gioco
Sync_11 My thinking was completely focused on the action Il mio pensiero era completamente focalizzato sull’azione
Strength Str_1 I was aware of my strengths Ero consapevole dei miei punti di forza
Str_2 I knew I could perfectly perform every move Sapevo di essere perfettamente in grado di eseguire ogni mossa
Str_7 I was afraid of not being able to tackle possible issues Avevo timore di non riuscire ad affrontare eventuali criticità
Str_8 I knew exactly which skills to employ at different times Sapevo esattamente quali abilità impiegare nei diversi momenti
Str_9 I was afraid of breaking me down when faced with the first difficulties Avevo paura di abbattermi di fronte alle prime difficoltà
Str_10 I felt that I could effectively deal with unexpected situations and problems Sentivo di poter affrontare efficacemente situazioni e problemi inaspettati
Str_11 I felt able to achieve the goals Mi sentivo in grado di raggiungere gli obiettivi
Energy Ene_1 I acted in a gritty way Agivo in maniera grintosa
Ene_3 I wasn’t able to let my energies flow Non riuscivo a far fluire le energie
Ene_4 I felt filled with grit for its entire duration Mi sono sentito pieno di grinta per tutta la sua durata
Ene_5 I knew how to regulate my forces Ho saputo regolare le mie forze
Ene_6 I felt determined Mi sentivo determinato
Ene_10 I was able to regulate my energies to perform at my best Riuscivo a regolare le mie energie per rendere al meglio
Rhythm Rhy_2 Thoughts, movements and commands came naturally to me Pensieri, movimenti e comandi mi venivano naturali
Rhy_3 I was awkward in my movements Ero impacciato nei movimenti
Rhy_5 I moved with the right timing Mi muovevo con il giusto tempismo
Rhy_6 I dedicated the necessary time to each action Ho dedicato a ciascuna azione il tempo necessario
Rhy_8 I did everything at the appropriate time Ho fatto ogni cosa al momento opportuno
Rhy_10 I was moving forward without indecision Andavo avanti senza indecisioni
Rhy_12 The rhythm of my actions was well suited to that situation Il ritmo con cui agivo era giusto per quella situazione
Activation Act_5 I couldn’t wait to get started Non vedevo l’ora di iniziare
Act_7 I felt that what I was doing had an important meaning for me Sentivo che ciò che facevo aveva un significato importante per me
Act_8 I felt I could fulfil myself by doing that activity Sentivo di potermi realizzare svolgendo quell’attività
Act_9 I felt proud of what I was doing Mi sentivo fiero di quello che facevo
Act_11 I felt motivated to reach the goals that I had set Ero motivato a raggiungere gli obiettivi che mi ero posto

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS

IGD = Internet Gaming Disorder
PST = Psychological Skill Training
CMV = Common Method Variance

REFERENCES

[1] Newzoo Company. Global Esports & Live Streaming Market Report 2021. Available from: https://newzoo.com/insights/trend-reports/newzoos-global-esports-live-streaming-market-report-2021-free-version/
[2] Leis O, Lautenbach F. Psychological and physiological stress in non-competitive and competitive esports settings: A systematic review. Psychol Sport Exerc 2020; 51: 101738.
[3] Adamus T. Playing computer games as electronic sport: In search of a theoretical framework for a new research field.Computer games and new media cultures: A handbook of digital games studies. Berlin: Springer 2012; pp. 477-90.
[4] Pedraza-Ramirez I, Musculus L, Raab M, Laborde S. Setting the scientific stage for esports psychology: a systematic review. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol 2020; 13(1): 319-52.
[5] Jonasson K, Thiborg J. Electronic sport and its impact on future sport. Sport Soc 2010; 13(2): 287-99.
[6] Hallmann K, Giel T. eSports – Competitive sports or recreational activity? Sport Manage Rev 2018; 21(1): 14-20.
[7] Summerley R. The development of sports: A comparative analysis of the early institutionalization of traditional sports and E-sports. Games Cult 2020; 15(1): 51-72.
[8] Parry J. E-sports are not sports. Sport Ethics Philos 2019; 13(1): 3-18.
[9] Borowy M, Jin D. Pioneering esport: The experience economy and the marketing of early 1980s arcade gaming contests. Int J Commun 2013; 7: 2254-74.
[10] Hamari J, Sjöblom M. What is eSports and why do people watch it? Internet Res 2017; 27(2): 211-32.
[11] Smithies TD, Toth AJ, Conroy E, Ramsbottom N, Kowal M, Campbell MJ. Life after esports: A grand field challenge. Front Psychol 2020; 11: 883.
[12] Rudolf K, Bickmann P, Froböse I, Tholl C, Wechsler K, Grieben C. Demographics and health behavior of video game and esports players in Germany: The esports study 2019. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020; 17(6): 1870.
[13] Boldi A, Rapp A, Tirassa M. Professionals, streamers and amateur players: An ethnography for exploring organizational behaviours in different work-play conditions. International Psychological Applications Conference and Trends. 410-4.
[14] Ward MR, Harmon AD. ESport superstars. J Sports Econ 2019; 20(8): 987-1013.
[15] Sharma MK, Palanichamy T, Sahu M, Kanchana DM. Influence of Esports on stress: A systematic review. Ind Psychiatry J 2020; 29(2): 191-9.
[16] Tholl C, Bickmann P, Wechsler K, Froböse I, Grieben C. Musculoskeletal disorders in video gamers – a systematic review. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 2022; 23(1): 678.
[17] Pérez-Rubio C, Gonzàlez J, Garcés de LFEJ. Personalidad y burnout en jugadores profesionales de e-sports. Cuad Psicol Deporte 2017; 17(1): 41-50.
[18] Carlisle K. Utility of DSM-5 criteria for internet gaming disorder. Psychol Rep 2021; 124(6): 2613-32.
[19] Himmelstein D, Liu Y, Shapiro JL. An exploration of mental skills among competitive league of legend players. Int J Gaming Comput-Mediated Simulations 2017; 9(2): 1-21. [IJGCMS].
[20] Zuluev EG. Using a Team-Based Approach to Psychological Skills Training with an Esports Team. Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2021; p. 8624.
[21] Daubert C. Esports mental performance.Handbook of Esports Medicine. Cham: Springer 2021.
[22] Collins TJ. Psychological Skills Training Manual for eSports Athletes Masters of Education in Human Movement. Sport, and Leisure Studies Graduate Projects 2017; p. 49.
[23] Trotter MG, Coulter TJ, Davis PA, Poulus DR, Polman R. Social support, self-regulation, and psychological skill use in e-athletes. Front Psychol 2021; 12: 722030.
[24] Poulus D, Coulter TJ, Trotter MG, Polman R. Stress and coping in esports and the influence of mental toughness. Front Psychol 2020; 11: 628.
[25] Vercelli G. Winning with the mind. Milan: Ponte alle Grazie 2005.
[26] Vercelli G. The S.F.E.R.A. Model.Baria A, Nabli EH, Madani M, Essiyedal A, Aragon M, Ouartassi E. Meeting New Challenges and Bridging Cultural Gaps in Sport and Exercise Psychology – Proceedings of ISSP 12th World Congress. Marrakech: International Society of Sport Psychology 2009.
[27] Vercelli G. L'intelligenza agonistica Affrontare le sfide nella vita, nel lavoro, nello sport. Ponte alle Grazie. Milano 2022.
[28] Dolce V, Borla Cart V, Vercelli G, Cortese CG. SFERA test: Preliminary validation with an athletes’ sample Proceedings of XIX EAWOP Congress “Working for the Greater Good”. Torino: European Association of Work and Organizational Psychology 2019; p. 1991. Available from: http://www.eawop.org/past-congresses
[29] Zito M, Cortese CG, Colombo L. The italian adaptation of the work-related flow inventory (WOLF) to sport: The I-WOLFS scale. Boll Psicol Appl 2018; 281(67): 38-45.
[30] Balducci C, Fraccaroli F, Schaufeli WB. Psychometric properties of the italian version of the utrecht work engagement scale (UWES-9): A cross-cultural analysis. Eur J Psychol Assess 2010; 26(2): 143-9.
[31] Bányai F, Griffiths MD, Király O, Demetrovics Z. The psychology of esports: A systematic literature review. J Gambl Stud 2019; 35(2): 351-65.
[32] Dolce V, Borla Cart V, Vercelli G, Cortese CGSFERA. test: Preliminary validation with an athletes’ sample. 19th EAWOP Congress: Working for the greater good: Inspiring people, designing jobs and leading organizations for a more inclusive society European Association of Work and Organizational Psychology (EAWOP) . Turin. 2019; p. 1990.
[33] Cortese CG, Dolce V, Borla Cart V, Loretoni I, Vercelli G. Caratteristiche psicometriche dello SFERA Business Test: primo studio su un campione di manager. Proceedings of XVI Congresso Nazionale della Sezione AIP di Psicologia per le organizzazioni. 110-1.
[34] Cortese CG, Emanuel F. Il docente universitario: Efficacia della prestazione a lezione e autoefficacia didattica 2022. Available from: https://library.oapen.org/bitstream/handle/20.500.12657/58551/1/9788835140726.pdf
[35] DeVellis RF. Scale Development: Theory and Applications. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE 2016.
[36] Bakker AB. The work-related flow inventory: Construction and initial validation of the WOLF. J Vocat Behav 2008; 72(3): 400-14.
[37] Schaufeli WB, Salanova M, González-romá V, Bakker AB. The measurement of engagement and burnout: A two sample confirmatory factor analytic approach. J Happiness Stud 2002; 3(1): 71-92.
[38] Spence JT, Robbins AS. Workaholism: Definition, measurement, and preliminary results. J Pers Assess 1992; 58(1): 160-78.
[39] Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee JY, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol 2003; 88(5): 879-903.
[40] Bandalos DL, Finney SJ. Factor analysis: Exploratory and confirmatory.The reviewer’s guide to quantitative methods in the social sciences. New York: Routledge 2010; pp. 93-114.
[41] Yuan KH, Bentler PM. Three likelihood-based methods for mean and covariance structure analysis with nonnormal missing data.Sociological methodology; American Sociological Association. Washington, DC 2000; pp. 165-200.
[42] Little TD. Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford press 2013.
[43] Brislin RW. Back-translation for cross-cultural research. J Cross Cult Psychol 1970; 1(3): 185-216.
[44] Nestler EJ, Carlezon WA Jr. The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59(12): 1151-9.
[45] Bandura A. Social Learning Theory. New York: General Learning Press 1971.
[46] Bandura A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev 1977; 84(2): 191-215.
[47] Vercelli G. SFERA: Un modello per il miglioramento continuo in psicologia dello sport. G Ital Psicol 2008; 1(2): 34-5.