RESEARCH ARTICLE


The Impact of Quarantine on Emotions during the COVID-19 Pandemic



Gintaras Chomentauskas1, *, Edita Dereškevičiūtė1, Gustė Kalanavičiūtė1, Rasa Ališauskienė2, Kristina Paulauskaitė1
1 Human Study Center, Vilnius, Lithuania
2 Baltic Surveys / The Gallup Organization, Vilnius, Lithuania


Article Metrics

CrossRef Citations:
0
Total Statistics:

Full-Text HTML Views: 2604
Abstract HTML Views: 768
PDF Downloads: 594
Total Views/Downloads: 3966
Unique Statistics:

Full-Text HTML Views: 1366
Abstract HTML Views: 369
PDF Downloads: 399
Total Views/Downloads: 2134



Creative Commons License
© 2021 Chomentauskas et al.

open-access license: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License (CC-BY 4.0), a copy of which is available at: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode. This license permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

* Address correspondence to this author at the Human Study Center, Vilnius, Lithuania; E-mail: gintaras@humanstudy.lt


Abstract

Introduction:

The present study explores how the general population of Lithuania felt during the national quarantine of COVID-19 in the period from March 30th to June 8th, 2020. Representative subjects were interviewed five times using adapted single-item questions to evaluate their emotions, stress, and perceived pain levels.

Methods:

It was hypothesized that gender and age-related differences would be found in emotional responses to the quarantine.

Results and Discussion:

During the five polls taken, more women were found to report feeling stressed, anxious, sad, and in more physical pain than men. Evaluations of anger, enjoyment, and calmness provided no statistically significant gender differences. Emotions were found to differ significantly between three age groups (18-29, 30-49, 50-74).

Conclusion:

Contrary to expectations, the biggest negative impact of quarantine was found in the youngest group aged 18-29 years: it showed the highest prevalence of stress, anxiety, and sadness of all age groups. Possible explanations for different gender and age-related emotional reactions are discussed.

Keywords: Coronavirus, COVID-19, Pandemic, Quarantine, Emotion dynamics, Emotional reactions.



1. INTRODUCTION

Quarantine and isolation help prevent the spread of contagious diseases by restricting people’s movements [1, 2]. Isolation separates sick people from healthy people, whilst quarantine segregates individuals exposed to the disease. Following the announcement that coronavirus (COVID-19) is a pandemic [3], countries started implementing national quarantines. China was the first to lock down their city of Wuhan, prohibiting any public movement and closing workplaces [4]. Similarly, strict measures were taken in Italy - the epicenter of the coronavirus in Europe - as the whole country went into lockdown [5]. Other countries with lower infection rates, like Lithuania had more lenient rules [6]. Schools, restaurants, and some other workplaces were closed, but public movement wearing face masks was allowed in most places.

Physical constraints of quarantine and social isolation of people have been found to create psychological distress [7]. Governments aim to control the pandemic, but they need guidance on dealing with people’s mental health. To provide effective recommendations, more research on the psychological impact of quarantine is needed. Useful insights into this were found during the SARS, Ebola, and MERS pandemics, that can be applied to coronavirus pandemic. However, each viral pandemic is said to be unique and to have diverse consequences; thus, more research, specifically on COVID-19, is needed. Unfortunately for researchers, COVID-19 has affected everyone’s lives and made it difficult to distinguish control groups of zero exposure. Additionally, a wide variety of national quarantine regulations limit the exploration of universal trends of emotional responses.

Other limitations mentioned in the review by Brooks et al. [7] are inappropriately used clinical diagnostic tools for assessment of quarantine’s impact, and the population samples assessed are poorly representative of the general population. For instance, Hawryluck et al. [8] used a post-traumatic stress disorder diagnostic scale to assess post-traumatic stress after the SARS pandemic, even though quarantine is not qualified as trauma in the DSM-5 diagnosis for PTSD. Such assessment of quarantine’s impact on people’s mental health could have missed out on less severe symptoms from this high-threshold clinical scale. In terms of study samples used, some had a small sample size [9] or participants were undergraduate students [10], while others assessed only healthcare workers [11-13]. So, in our study, we sampled a representative group of Lithuanian citizens between 18 to 74 years of age. Single item questions for selected emotions were used as part of an opinion poll in fives waves of research covering a time span of 70 days. This allowed us to assess the dynamic of emotions in different genders and age groups that represent the general population of Lithuania.

1.1. Impact of Quarantine and Pandemics on Mental Health and Prevalent Emotions

Three major trends emerged from studies on emotions and mental health during previous pandemics: people show increased anxiety, depression, and stress symptoms. A study by Liu et al. [11] explored how the SARS outbreak in Beijing had influenced medical staff‘s psychopathology for 3 years post-pandemic. In one study, physicians and nurses who had high pandemic exposure were split into two groups based on having been quarantined or not, and were surveyed using the Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) [14] and the Impact of Event Scale-Revised (IES-R) [15] for PTSD symptoms. Health care workers studied in the work by Liu et al. [11] had been quarantined for around 10 days together with SARS patients and were taking care of them. They could not see their families and were at high risk of infection, which could have inflated their psychopathology symptoms throughout time. They were tested three years after that period. It appeared that participants who had been quarantined and had previous traumatic experiences had higher depressive symptoms. Being 35 years old or younger and being single were also predictive of higher depressive symptoms post-pandemic.

Anxiety increase was recorded and analyzed by Jeong et al. [16] in Korea during isolation periods of MERS and for 4-6 months afterward. People infected with MERS were compared against healthy but isolated people on the Generalized Anxiety Disorder scale (GAD-7) [17] and the Korean version of the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI) [18]. During isolation, 47.2% of MERS patients and 7.6% of quarantined people had anxiety symptoms, when general population prevalence was around 3.3% [16]. After 4-6 months post-pandemic, numbers decreased to 19.4% for MERS patients and 3.0% for isolated healthy people, showing possible recovery to normal anxiety levels. Anger trends were similar to anxiety, but with a bigger decrease. However, Jeong et al. [16] reported that around 30% of people did not agree to answer questions on the phone and were swearing at the researchers. If this had been included in the data analysis, it could have shown higher anger results in the study.

The prominent psychological impact of quarantine is increased stress symptoms, as explored by Hawryluck et al. [8]. A web-based survey with questions from PTSD (IES-R [15];) and depression (CES-D [14];) scales were conducted in Toronto, Canada, around 36 days after SARS quarantine. More than half of respondents were healthcare workers aged 26-45, the majority having college-level education or higher, and most had medium to high levels of income. The isolation period was on average 10 days, and survey respondents were questioned on knowledge and understanding of the reasons for the quarantine, what they knew about infection control measures and where they found that information. Researchers used cut-off points that clinically diagnose PTSD and depression disorders and found that 28.9% and 31.2% of quarantined people met the criteria for PTSD and depression accordingly. Depressive and PTSD symptoms were strongly correlated together (r = 0.78) and an increase of both was predicted by household income - lower-income led to more pronounced symptoms. Neither age, level of education, nor career choice had any influence, although a longer duration of quarantine significantly predicted higher PTSD symptoms. No such trend was found with depression levels, however, the study participants were isolated only for around 10 days and we might suppose that different results could emerge from longer quarantine periods.

The aforementioned findings of increased anxiety, depression, and PTSD symptoms due to quarantine or pandemics generally might depend on certain risk factors. According to Brooks et al. [7], in an extensive literature review, healthcare workers were more severely distressed than the general public, especially if they were quarantined. In fact, most studies report higher risks for healthcare workers during pandemics, but most of them do not compare their results with the results of the general public. The study that did so found no significant correlations between being a healthcare worker and showing greater PTSD symptoms [8]. This could be because healthcare workers had a better understanding of quarantine measures and had a better understanding of the situation. Another risk factor mentioned by some researchers [16] is a history of mental illness. People who have pre-existing mental conditions are more at risk during quarantine, as they have weak emotional control and rely more on social support. This implies that they require additional support to protect their mental health during quarantine and isolation.

Some studies suggest that the elderly are at the greatest risk, not only because of being in the COVID-19 health risk group, but also because they are more susceptible to depression and anxiety. A longitudinal study found a pattern of social disconnectedness predicting perceived isolation which in turn predicts depression and anxiety symptoms [19]. In other words, having a smaller social network or fewer social interactions leads to loneliness and a perceived lack of support, which predict the severity of depression and anxiety. Due to coronavirus regulations to stay at home, the elderly are at greater risk of feeling lonely, unsupported, depressed and anxious about themselves and others. An alarming finding from some researchers [20] suggests that perceived social isolation has the same detrimental effects as real physical isolation. Their experiments with isolated non-human social species show multiple physical damages, especially on the HPA axis that regulates sleep, mood, and stress. So, since the elderly were put at actual physical isolation and perhaps already had fewer social interactions, the risk only increases.

1.2. Stressors during and after Quarantine

Stressors for emotional distress resulting from quarantine have been summarized in a systematic review [7]. Duration of quarantine is one of the factors influencing PTSD symptoms, anger, and avoidance behaviors as seen from comparisons of people quarantined for more or less than 10 days. It is yet unknown how long-term COVID-19 quarantines of 2 months or longer will impact people’s emotions. During the SARS pandemic, people felt bored and frustrated in home isolation, having restricted contact with loved ones, with routine activities coming to a halt. Psychological distress has been intensified due to inadequate supplies of food, water, and accommodation for some people, as well as restricted to medical care not related to SARS. People were angry and confused when responsible healthcare authorities spread inadequate information and unclear guidelines during the SARS pandemic. Lack of transparency from such authorities and explanation for their course of actions led to violation of quarantine rules and it decreased the public trust [7].

One of the major factors influencing psychological distress is financial losses due to quarantine as people are unable to work, have little savings, and so on. It is also a risk factor for increased anger and anxiety symptoms, lasting months after quarantine [16]. Hawryluck et al. [8] found that low income before quarantine predicts higher PTSD and depression symptoms, so it could be assumed that these people might experience even higher symptoms by losing money during quarantine. They have more to lose as their household incomes are lower, and it predicts higher psychological distress [7].

The stigma surrounding risk groups and people of high exposure to the virus adds to psychological distress after quarantine. Multiple studies mentioned the [7] review have found stigmatization of people who have been quarantined during previous SARS outbreaks. Participants reported people avoiding them, treating them with fear and suspicion, making comments about them. Similarly, during the Ebola outbreak, some people were unable to resume their jobs as it seemed too risky for them or because colleagues expressed fear of contagion.

1.3. Gender Differences

Multiple studies have observed gender differences in the prevalence and severity of depression and anxiety symptoms. Globally the female to male ratio is 1.7:1 as women are almost twice as likely to be diagnosed with depression than men [21]. Multiple studies across countries find similar prevalence, indicating that biological sex differences have more influence than socio-economic factors. The main explanation is that women experience hormonal fluctuations across life during the stages of puberty, menstruation, pregnancy, and menopause, which could trigger the onset of depression. Women are more emotional and report depression symptoms more often than men, who externalize their feelings in such ways as anger or misuse of alcohol [21]. Gender roles are important, as men and women have different social needs, which result in different behaviors when unmet. Women report higher loneliness and depressive symptoms as their social needs are more complex and demanding [22]. Meanwhile, men are more reluctant to directly admit to feeling lonely as they grow up and they are expected to be more socially independent.

An electroencephalography (EEG) study [23] found gender differences in a threat startle paradigm measuring neural-level sensitivity and its link to anxiety and panic symptoms. It was found that women have higher startle potentiation in both predictable and unpredictable threat conditions compared to men. Females also reported higher panic symptoms in anticipation of an unpredictable threat, showing higher anxiety levels concerning unpredictable future events. Across both genders, greater panic symptoms were associated with higher startle potentiation in an unpredictable threat condition, but no significant link was found in the predictable condition. This indicates that genders differ in panic symptoms experiences because they show different startle reflex in anticipation of unpredictable physical threats. Yet, both men and women with heightened sensitivity to unpredictable threats can show greater panic and anxiety symptoms due to increased startle potentiation. Burani and Nelson [23] did not give any suggestions for follow-up studies, but we think their findings might be put to the test by researching gender differences in emotional reactions during COVID-19.

1.4. Age

A longitudinal study of the elderly aged 57-85 years found associations between social isolation and disconnectedness on anxiety and depression symptoms [19]. The fewer social connections and activities they had, the more anxious and depressed they felt. Even though no younger people were recruited for comparison in this study, it still shows an increased risk for the elderly to be more anxious and depressed. However, not only do older people feel lonely and sad, more than half of the adolescents say they “sometimes feel lonely” too [22]. During adolescence, youngsters explore which friendships work, how their expectations are met, and so on. Yet the elderly, aged 80 years and more, feel lonely more often, as their personal networks shrink with time. Feelings of loneliness serve an evolutionary function to motivate people to reconnect with others. It motivates both young and old people to be more socially active, seek out support when needed and enjoy life by sharing it with others.

Interesting differences in PTSD prevalence were observed across the lifespan of men and women [24]. The highest prevalence of PTSD in men was at 41-45 years of age, for women at 51-55 years, whilst for both, the lowest was at 71-75 years. Men have quite stable levels of PTSD risk, yet it peaks in the early 40s as they usually take on more responsibilities in life (work, family, etc.) or potentially reach a midlife crisis. Women have an increased risk around the early 20s - a time of frequent changes (career, pregnancy) combined with social pressures on “being a modern woman“. Both genders aged 60 and above start showing a decline in PTSD risk as they are thought to accept their imminent death and use early life experiences to cope with it. Using the wisdom of retrospect, they usually solve the crisis between ego and despair of old age, which shows in lower PTSD symptoms [24].

1.5. Emotion Dynamics during COVID-19 Pandemic

The negative consequences of quarantine are increased anxiety, depression, and stress symptoms, which might last long after the quarantine ends. Nine percent of health-care workers remained highly depressed 3 years after SARS quarantine [11]; MERS patients and healthy quarantined people stayed anxious 4-6 months after isolation [22]; up to 36 days after SARS quarantine, high PTSD symptoms were still prevalent amongst the general population [8]. Longitudinal studies on emotional change during the COVID-19 pandemic are still in progress, although China has shared its first analysis. Wang et al. [10] had surveyed the general Chinese population once when the cases of infections were rapidly increasing, and a month later when they were declining. More than 1,700 people responded to the survey, yet only around 300 people responded both times; remaining people responded once, either first or second time. Stress, anxiety, depression, and PTSD symptoms were measured using the IES-R [15] and Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS-21) [25] as in previous studies, yet contrary results were found. None of the scores, except for the PTSD scale, differed significantly between the first and second time of questioning. Only PTSD scores were significantly lower the second time, yet both times they were high enough to indicate a strong presence of symptoms. The researchers assumed that it was probably due to rapid and decisive measures taken by the Chinese government that people’s emotions remained stable [10]. Additionally, people aged 12 to 21.4 years had higher PTSD scores than those aged 49.6 to 59 years. Students reported struggling to study remotely, expressed uncertainty and stress about their school or college exams.

An explorative paper has been issued by early-career psychiatrists proposing an emotional curve framework that predicts emotion change during COVID-19 [26]. Young practitioners from World Health Organization regions discussed their countries’ mental health systems, preparations for coronavirus, economic plans, etc. An extensive literature review was combined with data gathered from country respondents, and a conceptual framework was created. After working on feedback, a final version of the emotional curve with double-peak emotional distress has been conceptualized. It shows initial distress to the pandemic, then community growing resilience, and another distress wave coming post-pandemic as mental disorder prevalence increases.

The most prominent consequences would be the aforementioned internal psychological struggles, such as anxiety, depression, and acute stress, as well as relapse to preexisting mental conditions. According to Ransing et al. [26], the second peak of distress comes either due to the second wave of COVID-19 cases, or people losing loved ones, or big economic and social disruptions happening. A phenomenon called ‘emotional contagion is also at play when people manifest their anxiety, anger or other moods across populations. It easily happens in today’s digitalized and global world, especially on social media platforms where people share their thoughts and feelings with thousands, indirectly ‘infecting’ them with their emotions.

1.6. Social Consequences and changes after Quarantine

China was the first country hit by COVID-19, revealing the impact it had on communities, psychologically and socially. A web-based study [27] surveyed China’s residents for their psychiatric symptoms and feelings of returning to work. Additionally to the diagnostic measures for anxiety, depression, and stress, questions on COVID-19 preventative hygiene recommendations and organizational measures were asked. Higher psychiatric symptoms were found to be associated with poor physical health and viewing return to work as a serious health hazard. As expected, adherence to hand hygiene recommendations, wearing face masks, and seeing safety improvements in workplaces meant people showed less severe psychiatric symptoms. Counterintuitively, returning to work did not result in increased severe stress, but workers group showed a much lower prevalence of symptoms compared to previous China‘s population surveys [27]. This could be because of very strict preventative and safety measures implemented nationally in China that contrast with much milder regulations when people resumed working again.

1.7. Present Study and Hypotheses

The present study explores how the general population of Lithuania felt during the COVID-19 pandemic during the national quarantine. From 30th of March to 8th of June, 2020 a representative group of Lithuanian citizens 18-74 years of age were surveyed five times on their emotional states. The goal of the current study was to research the emotional reactions and changes of the population of Lithuania during quarantine.

Based on previous studies, we expected to find certain trends in emotional reactions to COVID-19, that is, different reactions between men and women and between age groups.

1.7.1. Hypothesis 1

Prevalence of negative feelings will be higher in females than males.

1.7.2. Hypothesis 2

prevalence of negative feelings will differ across age groups, with older people having more negative feelings than younger people.

2. METHODS

2.1. Procedure and Participants

In total, 2634 participants (1536 females and 1098 males) aged 18 to 74 years (M = 42.94, SD = 13.13) were interviewed. National representative samples of Lithuanians were surveyed and analyzed, grouped into 18-29 years old group (N = 496), 30-49 years group (N = 1159), and 50-74 group (N = 979). In each wave of survey different representative sample of Lithuania, inhabitants were interviewed. During the time span of 70 days in lockdown, surveys were completed five times (each poll ending date March 30th, April 8th, April 30th, May 21st, June 8th) using a computer-assisted web interviewing method. Questions from Gallup’s Global Emotions Report [1] were used to evaluate emotions, stress and pain: “Did you experience the following feelings most of the day yesterday? How about physical pain?”. Correspondingly questions about anxiety, sadness, stress, anger, enjoyment, calmness and physical pain were asked for evaluation. Demographic information of age and gender was collected as well. Emotion states were explored and compared across five waves of survey.

2.2. Data Analysis Procedure

We had two predictor variables of gender (male, female) and three independent variables of age (18-29, 30-49, 50-74) based on demographic information provided by participants. There were seven dependent variables of self-reported emotions and states, including anxiety, sadness, anger, enjoyment, calmness, stress, and physical pain.

A between-subjects t-test was run on SPSS V.26 to compare male and female answers to each feeling question. One-way ANOVA with Tukey’s HSD post hoc test was used to compare three age groups on the same answers.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Stress

Across the five polls and all age groups, significantly more females (M = 0.43, SD = 0.495) than males (M = 0.37, SD = 0.484) expressed feeling stressed, t(2632) = -2.86, p < 0.05.

One-way ANOVA showed that stress differed statistically significantly between different age groups, F(2, 2631) = 31.04, p < 0.001. A Tukey HSD post hoc revealed that stress statistically decreased going upwards from the 18-29 age group (M = 0.53, SD = 0.499) to the 30-49 age group (M = 0.42, SD = 0.494) and to the 50-74 age group (M = 0.32, SD = 0.269), p < 0.001 (Figs. 1 and 2).

Fig. (1). Stress prevalence between males and females.

Fig. (2). Stress prevalence across different age groups and poll times.

Fig. (3). Anxiety prevalence of males and females across.

Fig. (4). Anxiety prevalence across age groups and poll times.

3.2. Anxiety

A similar trend was found on anxiety, as significantly more females (M = 0.53, SD = 0.49) than males (M = 0.43, SD = 0.496) reported feeling anxious, t(2632) = -5.08, p < .001.

One-way ANOVA revealed that anxiety differed statistically significantly between different age groups, F(2, 2631) = 4.09, p < .05. Anxiety rates were significantly higher in the 18-29 age group (M = 0.55, SD = 0.49) than in 30-49 (M = 0.47, SD = 0.50) and 50-74 (M = 0.48, SD = 0.50) groups, p < .05. However, the differences between 30-49 and 50-74 age groups were not statistically significant, p = .951 (Figs. 3 and 4).

3.3. Sadness

Once again females (M = 0.45, SD = 0.49) had significantly higher sadness rates than males (M = 0.36, SD = 0.43), t(2632) = -4.84, p < .001.

One-way ANOVA showed that sadness differed statistically significantly between different age groups, F(2, 2631) = 5.75, p < .01. The 18-29 age group (M = 0.48, SD = 0.50) had significantly highest sadness rates compared to the 30-49 age group (M = 0.39, SD = 0.49; p = .003) and the 50-74 age group (M = 0.40, SD = 0.50; p = .010). The differences between 30-49 and 50-74 age groups were not statistically significant p = .956 (Figs. 5 and 6).

3.4. Anger

The prevalence of anger between females (M = 0.34, SD = 0.47) and males (M = 0.33, SD = 0.48) did not differ statistically significantly p = .491. However, the anger scores of respondents of different ages varied significantly, F(2, 2631) = 14.07, p < .001. The 18-29 age group (M = 0.38, SD = 0.49) had similar rates as 30-49 age group (M = 0.37, SD = 0.48) and both groups significantly differed from 50-74 group (M = 0.32, SD = 0.47, p < .001) (Figs. 7 and 8).

Fig. (5). Sadness prevalence of males and females.

Fig. (6). Sadness prevalence across age groups and poll times.

Fig. (7). Anger prevalence of males and females.

Fig. (8). Anger prevalence across age groups and poll times.

3.5. Enjoyment

No significant differences were found between males (M = 0.60, SD = 0.49) and females (M = 0.62, SD = 0.49) on enjoyment rates, t(2632) = -.78, p = .434. In age comparison emerged one significant (p = .004) difference between 30-49 group (M = 0.65, SD = 0.48) and 50-74 group (M = 0.58, SD = 0.49). Effect of age was significant overall F(2, 2631) = 5.26, p = .005, despite 18-29 (M = 0.61, 0.49) group showing no significant differences with other groups (Figs. 9 and 10).

Fig. (9). Enjoyment prevalence between males and females.

Fig. (10). Enjoyment prevalence between age groups and across poll times.

3.6. Calmness

No significant differences were found between males (M = 0.64, SD = 0.48) and females (M = 0.61, SD = 0.49) on calmness prevalence, t(2632) = 1.58, p = .114.

One-way ANOVA showed a significant age effect on calmness prevalence F(2, 2631) = 3.35, p = .035, however the post hoc test did not reveal between which groups it was. The averages for calmness prevalence are shown below (Figs. 11 and 12).

3.7. Physical Pain

More females (M = 0.23, SD = 0.42) reported experiencing physical pain more than males (M = 0.19, SD = 0.39), t(2632) = -2.15, p = .032.

Effect of age was significant F(2, 2631) = 6.49, p = .002 but only between 30-49 age group (M = 0.19, SD = 0.39) and 50-74 age group (M = 0.25, SD = 0.44). The 18-29 age group (M = 0.20, SD = 0.40) did not differ significantly from either group (Figs. 13 and 14).

4. DISCUSSION

The present study hypothesized that females will show a higher prevalence of negative feelings than males and that this prevalence will differ across age groups. The first hypothesis had been partially supported as women reported higher prevalence than men on stress, anxiety, sadness, and physical pain. Anger prevalence difference between genders was not statistically significant. The second hypothesis was also supported as we found significant differences between certain age groups on stress, anxiety, sadness, and anger. For example, we found that stress gradually decreased with age and anxiety differed between youngest and oldest groups. Also, sadness was highest in the youngest group while middle-aged and oldest did not differ significantly, and anger decreased with age as well.

Fig. (11). Calmness prevalence between males and females.

Fig. (12). Calmness prevalence across age groups and poll times.

Fig. (13). Physical pain prevalence between males and females.

Fig. (14). Physical pain prevalence across age groups and poll times.

Additionally, we have found a trend of positive emotions as calmness and enjoyment increased for both genders and all age groups throughout the survey waves. A similar trend was found in the United States on the positive impact of quarantine and it will be compared to our findings and discussion of possible explanations. To explain our findings on negative emotion trends, studies on gender differences and age in relation to mental health issues will be discussed.

4.1. Gender Differences

We found that females reported a higher prevalence of stress, anxiety, sadness, and physical pain than males over the course of our survey. Women also showed slightly more anger, but the effect was not statistically significant. Women also showed slightly more anger, but the effect was not statistically significant. Similar studies across European countries found gender differences in emotional response to the COVID-19 pandemic as well, with women presenting more severe anxiety, depression, and acute stress levels [28]. A Spanish study by Garcia-Fernandez et al. [28] also explored domestic violence occurrences and found a link between reported incidents and more severe depression for women. Some explain the increased violence at home resulting from lost jobs and decreased financial security, which makes men more controlling over their wives, especially when they are unemployed and the family is left without income [29]. Even without the violence, with men working from home and children studying from home too, women can feel burdened to be full-time family caregivers. Women might feel exhausted from looking after their family and working themselves from home, creating a risk for burnout.

A different approach to explaining such gender differences in negative wellbeing is discussing social gender roles and how that can impact emotional responses to quarantine. Men are raised to be socially independent and solve problems themselves without relying too much on others [22], whereas women seek out support from others more often than men do. Because of this, women felt more sad and anxious when their social interactions were restricted by the quarantine rules. Men struggle as well with negative feelings; they tend to deal with them on their own and seek help less often than females. A review by Albert [21] supports our finding that females feel sadder and that they are two times more often diagnosed with depression than men. Anxiety disorder is also more often diagnosed for women, as shown by Regier, Narrow and Rae [30] because females tend to internalize negative feelings more than males. A tendency of women to react more sensitively to stress than males was also found and explained by Matud [31]. It is also interesting how from the beginning of our survey, women’s anxiety rates were increasing with time, while that of men, the rate was gradually decreasing. This could mean that men somehow adjusted better than women.

Differences in gender-related stress response can be due to differences in coping styles. Coping is the cognitive and behavioral response taken when faced with stressful situations or threats and is aimed at reducing distress. This can be avoidance or detachment and it can be problem-focused or emotion-oriented. Problem-focused coping includes cognitive and behavioral attempts to modify or eliminate the stressful situation, while emotion-oriented coping is aimed at changing emotional responses in the situation. Studies have suggested that the latter coping style is less effective and more associated with higher psychological distress [31]. There have also been gender differences in coping styles in stressful situations. Matud [31] has sampled over two thousand males and females to explore gender differences in stress prevalence, and in coping styles used. Various standardized questionnaires on stress were administered to differentiate how certain situations were more stressful for men or women. Women were found to more frequently use emotion-focused and avoidance coping than men who used rational and detachment style coping.

4.2. Age Differences

The participants aged 18-29 showed the highest prevalence of stress, anxiety and sadness compared to other age groups. Anger was similarly more prevalent in the youngest and middle-aged group of 30-49 year-olds. The oldest group aged 50-74 had the highest physical pain reports but did not dominate on other emotions evaluated. An interesting trend was found in the 50-74 group: Initially, they had the highest anxiety levels, but these rapidly diminished over the months of quarantine. Such a trend could be explained by different life experiences and different abilities to rationally cope with and adapt to threats such as COVID-19. We can speculate, that the oldest group has experienced a variety of social hardships in their lifetime and lived through different epidemic instances. This gives them the possibility to rationally evaluate threats and use more adaptive psychological strategies to deal with COVID-19. Thus, the oldest group, despite objectively being in a bigger danger [3], paradoxically had experienced the least amount of stress and negative emotions.

Another explanation for differences between age groups in our study could be due to differences in age-related social needs. The 18 -29 and 30-49 year-olds are more socially active and have bigger social networks than people of older age because social connections tend to shrink with time. This is why the implied social isolation during quarantine possibly does bigger emotional harm to these groups by restricting their social activities. There could be more factors at play. One is disruption of habitual education and/or work processes. Chinese students reported high stress levels during COVID-19 lockdown as they had to adjust to remote lessons and exams [10]. The middle-aged, most economically active group had financial difficulties, as some lost their jobs or struggled to work remotely. During SARS outbreak, the lowest household income predicted high stress and depression rates because people struggled to support their families [8]. Changed work routines can create tension and conflicts at home, limit financial freedom, and lead to future worries [8]. Thus it leads to more stress and anxiety for socially active and working people to be isolated at home than it does for people in retirement or nursing homes.

4.3. Similar Trends

A study like ours was done in the US by the Gallup group to explore the emotional well-being of the general US population during national quarantine. They surveyed people from the end of March to the end of May using online polls and found similar trends to ours. Initial stress, worry, anger, and other feelings were prominent; especially females reported higher negativity than males [32], just as we have found. In their latest interview data taken on April 27th-May 10th, 2020 more reports on happiness were found comparing to data from a month before [32]. No gender or age differences were reported for this finding, so similar to ours, it could be widespread across the whole population sample.

Brenan [33] did not interpret the improved emotional states of respondents but did mention that he interviewed during the period when when stay-at-home orders were being lifted in some states and people began feeling happier, expecting that their isolation was coming to an end.

Findings in our study show increased calmness and enjoyment without social context that would raise expectations that quarantine will be lifted soon. This suggests other possible explanations. We speculate that changes in routine and decreased work stress by itself could have led to mood improvements for some people. Perhaps, working remotely from the comfort of home, and reduced work stress, allowed some people to improve their routine, sleep and eating habits and gave them a chance to balance their life. Some people might have enjoyed spending more time with their family, or having more time for themselves, which made their days more comfortable and enjoyable.

5. LIMITATIONS AND IMPLICATIONS

Our study has provided insight into how the general population of Lithuania was feeling during the 2.5 month period of the first quarantine June 3rd, 2020 during the COVID-19 pandemic. We have found gender differences in the prevalence of negative emotions where women reported them more often than men. We also found how people of different ages reacted to the quarantine, revealing that the youngest participants felt the most distressed. These findings suggest that gender-specific and age-specific psychological and social assistance might be needed during a pandemic.

CONCLUSION

This study, however, is subject to some limitations. Firstly, using a single question scale to measure emotions might lack validity. However, standardized multiple item questionnaires are hardly applicable in general population research because of their high cost and wide scope. Secondly, we were limited in a time span of 2.5 months. The impact of COVID-19 is not over, and we might see different dynamics of emotions in months to come. Nonetheless, our findings provide an interesting picture of the dynamics of emotional reactions during quarantine in the general population and how they differ depending on gender and age. Our findings might provide useful insights for further psychological research and serve as an impulse to look for gender-specific and age-specific specific social and psychological support actions during pandemics.

ETHICAL STATEMENT

This study was not approved by any ethics committee. It was done according to general requirements for social surveys:

1. ICC/ESOMAR International Code on Market and Social Research.

2. GDPR requirements for consent on collecting private data.

The study was conducted according to the Helsinki principles.

CONSENT FOR PUBLICATION

Not applicable.

AVAILABILITY OF DATA AND MATERIALS

Not applicable.

FUNDING

None.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare no conflict of interest, financial or otherwise.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Declared none.

REFERENCES

[1] Gallup. Gallup Global Emotions 2019.
[2] Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Quarantine and isolation. 2020. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/quarantine/index.html
[3] WHO. WHO announces COVID-19 outbreak a pandemic. 2020. Available from: https://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/health-emergencies/coronavirus-covid-19/news/news/2020/3/who-announces-covid-19-outbreak-a-pandemic
[4] Sommerville. Coronavirus China: When did Wuhan lockdown begin and for how long?. 2020.
[5] Italian Government. Coronavirus, Conte signs the Dpcm March 11, 2020. Available from: https://www.garda.com/crisis24/news-alerts/321986/italy-prime-minister-announces-more-nationwide-lockdown-measures-march-11-update-17
[6] Government of the Republic of Lithuania. Nutarimas dėl karantino Lietuvos Respublikos teritorijoje paskelbimo (Nr 5466) 2020.
[7] Brooks S K, Webster RK, Smith LE, et al. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. The lancet 2020; 395(10227): 912-20.
[8] Hawryluck L, Gold WL, Robinson S, Pogorski S, Galea S, Styra R. Psychological effects of quarantine for SARS 2004. Available from: www.cdc.gov/eid
[9] Pan PJD, Chang SH, Yu YY. A support group for home-quarantined college students exposed to SARS: Learning from practice. J Spec Group Work 2005; 30(4): 363-74.
[10] Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, et al. A longitudinal study on the mental health of general population during the COVID-19 epidemic in China. Brain Behav Immun 2020; 87: 40-8.
[11] Liu X, Kakade M, Fuller CJ, et al. Depression after exposure to stressful events: lessons learned from the severe acute respiratory syndrome epidemic. Compr Psychiatry 2012; 53(1): 15-23.
[12] Wu P, Fang Y, Guan Z, et al. The psychological impact of the SARS epidemic on hospital employees in China: Exposure, risk perception, and altruistic acceptance of risk. Can J Psychiatry 2009; 54(5): 302-11.
[13] Robertson E, Hershenfield K, Grace SL, Stewart DE. The psychosocial effects of being quarantined following exposure to SARS: A qualitative study of Toronto health care workers. Can J Psychiatry 2004; 49(6): 403-7.
[14] Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1977; 1(3): 385-401.
[15] Weiss DS. The impact of event scale: Revised.Cross-cultural assessment of psychological trauma and PTSD 2007; 219-38.
[16] Jeong H, Yim HW, Song YJ, et al. Mental health status of people isolated due to Middle East Respiratory Syndrome. Epidemiol Health 2016; 38e2016048
[17] Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Löwe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med 2006; 166(10): 1092-7.
[18] Chon KK, Hahn DW, Lee CH, Speilberger CD. Korean adaptation of the state-trait anger expression inventory: Anger and blood pressure. Korean J Heal Psychol 1997; 2(1): 60-78.
[19] Santini ZI, Jose PE, York Cornwell E, et al. Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and symptoms of depression and anxiety among older Americans (NSHAP): a longitudinal mediation analysis. Lancet Public Health 2020; 5(1): e62-70.
[20] Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Norman GJ, Berntson GG. Social isolation. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2011; 1231(1): 17-22.
[21] Albert PR. Why is depression more prevalent in women? J Psychiatry Neurosci 2015; 40(4): 219-21.
[22] de Jong Gierveld J, van Tilburg TG, Dykstra PA. CHAPTER 27 loneliness and social isolation. Cambridge Handb Pers Relatsh 2016. Available from: https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/67bc/66d9f7d27349de61b163bdddd8b6557d15ae.pdf
[23] Burani K, Nelson B D. Gender differences in anxiety: The mediating role of sensitivity to unpredictable threat. Int J Psychophysiol 2020; 153: 127-34.
[24] Ditlevsen DN, Elklit A. The combined effect of gender and age on post traumatic stress disorder: Do men and women show differences in the lifespan distribution of the disorder? Ann Gen Psychiatry 2010; 9: 32.
[25] Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav Res Ther 1995; 33(3): 335-43.
[26] Ransing R, Adiukwu F, Pereira-Sanchez V, et al. Mental Health Interventions during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Conceptual Framework by Early Career Psychiatrists. Asian J Psychiatr 2020; 51(April)102085
[27] Tan W, Hao F, McIntyre RS, et al. Is returning to work during the COVID-19 pandemic stressful? A study on immediate mental health status and psychoneuroimmunity prevention measures of Chinese workforce. Brain Behav Immun 2020; 87(April): 84-92.
[28] García-Fernández L, Romero-Ferreiro V, Padilla S, David López-Roldán P, Monzó-García M, Rodriguez-Jimenez R. Gender differences in emotional response to the COVID-19 outbreak in Spain. Brain Behav 2021; 11(1)e01934
[29] Vora M, Malatesh BC, Das S, Chatterjee S. COVID-19 and domestic violence against women Asian J Psychiatr 2020; 53 Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7295494/pdf/main.pdf
[30] Regier DA, Narrow WE, Rae DS. The epidemiology of anxiety disorders: the Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) experience. J Psychiatr Res 1990; 24(Suppl. 2): 3-14.
[31] Matud MP. Gender differences in stress and coping styles. Pers Individ Dif 2004; 37(7): 1401-15.
[32] Brenan M. US Adults Report Less Worry, More Happiness 2020. Available from: https://news.gallup.com/poll/311135/adults-report-less-worry-happiness.aspx